Behavioral OMS Model
v1.4
Z.I. Wang, Ph.D.1,3 and L.F.
DellÕOsso, Ph.D.1-3
From
the Daroff-DellÕOsso Ocular Motility Laboratory1, Louis Stokes
Cleveland DVA Medical Center and Depts. of Neurology2 and Biomedical
Engineering3, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland OH, USA
OMLAB Report #070108
Written: 1/2/08; Placed on
Web Page: 7/1/08;
Last Modified:
7/1/08
Downloaded from: OMLAB.ORG
Send
questions, comments, and suggestions to: lfd@case.edu
This
work was supported in part by the Office of Research and Development, Medical
Research Service, Department of Veterans Affairs.
This
document is based on and updated from the OMS_Model_Notes text file from the
previous release of the OMS model (v1.0), available from http://www.omlab.org/OMLAB.html.
GENERAL
The
behavioral Ocular Motor System (OMS) model contains Saccadic, Smooth Pursuit
(SP), and Fixation subsystems controlled by an Internal Monitor (IM). It is
capable of simulating normal ocular motor responses to commonly applied target
stimuli (pulses, steps, ramps, and
step-ramps) as well as behaviors to these same stimuli by subjects with various
ocular motor system disorders. Among the latter are infantile nystagmus
syndrome (INS, aka CN), fusion maldevelopment nystagmus syndrome (FMNS, aka
LMLN), gaze-evoked nystagmus (GEN), myasthenia gravis (MG), and various types
of saccadic intrusions and oscillations (1-3).
The
underlying hypothesis of the OMS model is that the brain reconstructs target
position and velocity from retinal signals and efference copy of motor output
signals and it is those reconstructed signals that are used to make decisions
controlling ocular motor responses, not simply retinal error. This internal
reconstruction provides signals that may be thought of as "perceived"
target position and velocity and that are responsible for oscillopsia or its
absence.
Realization
of the actual functional block to accomplish each required motor or decision
signal is only one of a very large set of solutions.
"Non-physiological" blocks can (and will) be replaced by more
physiological designs without changing the overall behavior of the model. As an
example of that, we have already updated the ocular motor neuron pathways in
this new release of the model (version 1_4) to conform to recent physiological
findings (4) (details will be discussed below). The
strength of the model lies in its organization and interconnecting feedback
pathways; these reflect current knowledge of the ocular motor system's
organization and our interpretations of the mathematical and decision-making
functions required to simulate the wide range of ocular motor behaviors
recorded from both normals and those with the specific disorders incorporated
into the model.
MAJOR
MODEL CHANGES
Here
we summarize the major changes made in the model since its first release, v1.0
(see APPENDIX):
1.
A Re-distributed OMS Efferent Delay
The
human OMS has ~200-250 ms latency to position error and ~100-150 ms latency to
velocity error. In our model, the delays responsible for these latencies were
distributed in different sub-systems. A major limitation of the distribution of
the delays in the previous model was a lumped 30 ms efferent delay placed
immediately before the plant, i.e., the latency from OMN firing to the induced
eye movements. That lumped delay had no adverse effect for the overall
performance to the input stimuli we employed, and the model produced accurate
behavioral outputs (2). However, the physiological value for
that delay, as reported by Robinson et al. in an electrical stimulation study,
is approximately 4-5 ms (5). Also, the VOR response, which will be
integrated to this model in the future, has a short latency of ~15 ms; the
lumped efferent delay of 30 ms would have made this short latency impossible to
realize. Therefore, one major improvement of this current model is the
re-distribution of the 30 ms efferent delay throughout the functional blocks of
the internal monitor, i.e., the ÒbrainsÓ of the model.
We
divided the 30 ms delay into 5 ms (the physiological value), placed between the
OMN and the plant, and the remaining 25 ms, re-distributed back into the
upstream functional blocks. The re-distribution work was non-trivial, because
of the multiple internal calculations using feedback signals for efference copy
of eye position and velocity. Furthermore, the timings for making comparisons
in the functional blocks of the internal monitor also had to be reset by ±25
ms, depending on where in each block the 25 ms delay was placed. These timing
changes will be described below for the specific blocks.
2.
A Built-in Gaze-angle Variation Mechanism
INS
patients exhibit gaze-angle variations with a similar morphology as those seen
in Vestibular Nystagmus (VN) and FMNS. To simulate the gaze-angle variations in
INS, we used the same AlexanderÕs law functional block in the Internal Monitor
of the OMS model (just as in the FMNS simulations (6)). That block utilized a reconstructed
eye-position signal that excluded the nystagmus signal, i.e., an indication of
Òwhere the eye should be.Ó The output of the AlexanderÕs law block was used to
modulate the INS baseline sine-wave oscillation (7). The modulation was enabled by a
variable gain in the PMC+ block. The ÒnullÓ position and broadness can now be
specified via the AlexanderÕs law block and simulations of the INS variation
with gaze angle of specific individuals easily made. Thus, by using the same
mechanism of AlexanderÕs law regulation as in VN and FMNS, INS gaze-angle
variation was successfully simulated without the need for additional functional
blocks. The resulting new version of the OMS model is more flexible and can
accurately simulate the large range of ÒnullÓ positions and broadness observed
in INS patients.
3.
A Separate Pathway for ÒFastÓ and ÒSlowÓ OMNs and a ÒTenotomyÓ Plant
We
have shown in a study of post-tenotomy saccades that saccadic eye movements
were not affected while slow-phase velocities were reduced after the tenotomy
procedure (8). The study implied that the plant gain
might be a nonlinear function of steady-state tension that was turned down by a
proprioceptive feedback loop post-tenotomy. Recently, the separation of slow
and fast eye-movement signals were demonstrated in a retrograde injection study
from BŸttner-EnneverÕs group (4). The retrograde transneuronal transfer
of rabies virus from the Òen grappeÓ endplates (innervating ÒslowÓ muscle
fibers) and Òen plaqueÓ endplates (innervating ÒfastÓ fibers) showed separate
connections to ÒfastÓ and ÒslowÓ OMNs. Those ÒslowÓ OMNs are involved in only
slow eye movements, (i.e., vergence and smooth pursuit), muscle length
stabilization, and fixation, whereas the ÒfastÓ OMNs participate in all eye
movements. Therefore, the ÒfastÓ muscle fibers may be involved in all eye
movements and the ÒslowÓ fibers in an on-line proprioceptive tension control
loop. If tenotomy only reduces the firing rate of the ÒslowÓ OMNs, lowering INS
slow-phase velocity, it is reasonable that the saccades remained unchanged.
To
incorporate the dual OMN paradigm in our model, we created functional blocks
for both the fast and the slow OMNs, and connected saccadic pulse generator output
to only the ÒfastÓ OMNs, while providing all other outputs to both OMNs. The
ratio of these split signals is currently set to be 50% each, although this
number could vary due to the percentage of non-twitch fibers and their
contribution to the final muscle force generation (presently unknown, Òa
subject for the godsÓ—J. BŸttner-Ennever, personal communication).
Instead of just one OMN output to the plant, we now have a ÒfastÓ OMN output
and a ÒslowÓ OMN output, both going to the plant.
With
the dual-OMN model design, a ÒtenotomyÓ plant is more easily realized. Since
the tenotomy exclusively reduces the slow-phase signals, we placed a reduction
coefficient in the plant for small-signal inputs. This coefficient is
idiosyncratic and could depend on the pre-surgical waveform quality and/or
afferent deficits (9,10). We do realize, and indeed have
hypothesized, that a more complicated proprioceptive feedback control probably
exists to reduce the nystagmus slow-phase components; more neuroanatomical and
neurophysiological research is needed to accurately simulate the gains and time
constants of that control system.
OTHER
MODEL FUNCTIONAL BLOCKS
The
model uses a 2-pole plant and the pulse generator (PG) height and width
functions are tuned for that plant. Since both the PGÕs resettable neural
integrator (NI) and the plant (with a tenotomy coefficient) are now different
than in v1.0, the pulse-height table and the pulse-width block are tuned
slightly differently.
The
ÒfastÓ OMN block only passes the NI signals in the absence of any PG signals.
Because the ÒslowÓ OMNs do not receive PG signals, they function as relays of
the NI signals.
The
NI only integrates the PG signal until the "nihold" signal from the
IM stops it. That happens when the NI output equals the desired eye position.
Eye-velocity signals from the IM are integrated by the NI and also passed
through a gain "k" (0.25) to the OMN.
The
SP system is modified from David A. Robinson to function in a position-based
OMS (11). It contains the internal feedback loop
"PMC+" (plus noise). Finally, both retinal error and retinal error
velocity signals are sent from the retina to the IM.
The
model is modular and allows for substitution of different ocular motor plants,
NIs, PGs, Fixation, and SP subsystems. The current NI is a leaky (25 sec) NI
with a positive feedback loop to compensate for the leak. The PG contains both
a height and width nonlinearity, with the width (PW) controlled by a local
resettable NI (12). The PG signal has both phasic and tonic
adjustable gains to allow for simulation of saccadic plasticity.
MODEL
USAGE
The
original model (v1.0) was developed in Simulink 2.2 (part of the MATLAB 5.2
release). We have updated the model in Simulink 7.0 (part of Matlab 7.5) and
found no significant differences in the model output. During the upgrading, we
grounded all the unconnected ports (as required) and changed some incompatible
parts (e.g., the integrator in the resettable neural integrator in PG) to their
compatible equivalents.
Before
using the OMS model, it is advisable to make a backup copy so that an unchanged
copy of each version is available. Common ocular motor input stimuli (pulses,
steps, ramps, step-ramps) can be provided to the OMS model from the sources
provided. The model requires that the simulation "Solver options" be
"Fixed-step" (Fixed step size of 0.001) and "ode-4
(Runge-Kutta)".
There
are also a number of externally settable OMS-model parameters that allow
simulation of different waveforms of INS and FMNS as well as normal behaviors.
These are incorporated into profiles accessed using "setmdlp", a
program with a graphical interface that allows the user to easily inspect and
modify these parameters. Place the program "setmdlp" in a directory
that is on the MATLAB path. The parameter folder Òmodelparams/v1_4Ó contains
all the parameters for the new release; this should be the folder that "setmdlp"
loads from. The names of the files in that folder indicate the waveform type of
the output and whether itÕs pre-tenotomy or post-tenotomy. If they are post,
the percentage of amplitude reduction is also indicated. These files contain
the necessary changes for various parameters in the model to enable behavioral
simulations of different states of the OMS.
The
program, "setmdlp" requires, and will try to create if necessary, a
directory called "omPrefs" in the same directory where MATLAB exists
(to find, type the command "matlabroot" from the MATLAB command
line). The program, "setmdlp" will try to read the file
"mdlParams.mat", which contains the last set of model parameters
used. If this file does not exist,
"setmdlp" will prompt the user to load an existing parameter set or
create a new set.
If
the user wishes to change any parameter, they simply type the new value in the
appropriate field and click the "Apply" button so the model will
recognize the update.
Modified
parameter sets can be stored anywhere desired, using the "Save as..."
button, and loaded using the "Load" button. We have included a set of
parameters covering normal function of the OMS model as well as several
deficits and forms of nystagmus.
We
included the Libraries of all functional blocks in ÒOMS Model LibÓ and various
sub libraries in some functional blocks, allowing the user to swap between the
different implementations. If during your testing you left test points or extra
model blocks unconnected, Simulink will complain about any block that has
unconnected inputs or outputs. The warnings that are issued at run time do not
affect the operation of the model, but can be distracting when they clutter up
the command window. Simply type "warning off" at the command line to
disable this behavior.
MODEL
INTERNAL DETAILS
PG
A
S/H circuit passes the saccadic motor command to both the PH and PW functions.
The output of the PH function is passed through a switch to the output of the
PG. The switch is controlled by the output of the PW circuitry. PG is also fed
back through a refractory latch to the S/H, placing it in the H mode until 50
ms after the end of the PG signal. The refractory latch initiates a low output
from the onset of the PG signal. The trailing edge of PG then starts the
refractory timing circuit that outputs a high signal to the S/H after 50 ms
(user-definable by setting ÒRefract LatchÓ value), allowing the next pulse to
be generated.
SP
The
SP system operates on reconstructed target velocity from the IM and outputs a
desired eye velocity signal. The forward path contains a LP filter, gain,
velocity saturation and a premotor circuit (PMC+). The PMC+ circuit has an acceleration
saturation and an integrator in a negative feedback loop. Provision is also
made for velocity noise. The PMC+ circuit controls the oscillatory behavior of
the SP system. We used a modulation factor for the PMC+ gain to simulate the
gaze-angle effect of INS. That factor came from the AlexanderÕs law functional
block in the IM (delayed by 25ms because of the re-distribution of delays). The
ÒVelocity DeadzoneÓ block in the PMC+ circuit eliminates the artificial ripples
at the beginning of the simulation only for normal ocular motor simulations. Those ripples
are characteristics of the circuit and are essential in generating sinusoidal
oscillations of nystagmus.
IM
This
is the heart of the model, as it is responsible for separating true target
position and motion from retinal image position and motion. It does this using
efference copy of both eye position and velocity commands and a target-change,
decision-making circuit. Other inputs to the IM are a light/dark (L/D) signal
and a tonic imbalance (TI) signal; the latter might be driven by any of a
number of mechanisms hypothesized to cause FMNS. The IM consists of several
major functional blocks: Target Change Detection, Sampled Target & Error
Reconstruction (utilizing a Model OMN and Plant), Target & Slip Velocity
Reconstruction (utilizing Model Velocity circuitry and Plant), Saccade and
Drift Blanking, Braking/Foveating Saccadic Logic, NI control, Alexander's Law
variation, and Saccade Enabling. This overall structure of the model (i.e., the
functional blocks) remains the same as the first release (v1.0). However,
several functional blocks were combined in an earlier, unreleased version
(v1.1, see APPENDIX) for simplicity and non-redundant use of the same input
signals. Thus, ÒSampled Target ReconstructionÓ and ÒSampled Error
ReconstructionÓ were combined into ÒSampled Target & Error ReconstructionÓ
and ÒSlip Velocity ReconstructionÓ
and ÒTarget Velocity ReconstructionÓ were combined into ÒTarget & Slip
Velocity Reconstruction.Ó Since the delay immediately before the plant was
reduced from 30 ms to 5 ms and the remaining 25 ms was redistributed in the
internal monitor, the amount of delay in each of the sub functional blocks
might differ from the first release.
Target
Change Detection: If retinal
error velocity (ÒRetErrVelÓ = Òedot50Ó) is greater than 950 deg/sec,
"tchng" goes high (value = 1), indicating that the target has jumped.
This signal is used in combination with "tvelchng", which goes high
(value = 1) if the derivative of reconstructed target velocity, ÒtvprÓ, is
greater than 950 deg/s2.
Sampled
Target & Error Reconstruction: Retinal error position (Òerr50Ó) is
summed with the efference copy of eye position (ÒeposspfÓ and ÒeposspsÓ) after
the latter is passed through a model of the OMN (OMN2) and 2-pole plant
yielding "ecpy80". Appropriate delays are in place. The resulting
signal is reconstructed target position (T'). T' is sampled to yield T'*
(ÒtpstarÓ) when either a target change (ÒtchngÓ) is detected or a retinal
feedback sample (ÒrfbsplÓ) is called for by the Saccade Enable circuitry.
Target
& Slip Velocity Reconstruction: Retinal error velocity (Òedot50Ó) is
delayed, limited and summed with ÒimvelÓ (output of the 1-zero, 2-pole
"Model Plant+"); the sum is then passed through a Sample and Hold
(based on the "sbsp" signal from the ÒSaccade and Drift BlankingÓ
block), a low-pass filter and a dead zone to yield reconstructed target
velocity (ÒTvel'*Ó), which is the input motor command signal "SPmc" =
"tvpr" to the SP circuitry. "tvpr" is summed with the
efference copy of eye velocity (ÒevelprÓ) and then summed with tonic imbalance
(TI). Appropriate delays are in place.
Saccade
Enable: This block contains the sub-blocks ÒEnable Control,Ó ÒRetinal
Feedback (RetFB) Enable & Sample,Ó and ÒDefoveating Fast-Phase Generation
(DFFPh Gen).Ó The Enable Control circuitry sends output signals to both RetFB
Enable & Sample circuits and its sub-block ECPY Timing. The first output
from RetFB Enable & Sample circuits directly enables a saccade to be initiated
(ÒRetFB EnableÓ prolonged = ÒsacenabÓ). The second output from the Saccade
Enabling (from RetFB Enable & Sample) is a retinal feedback sample (ÒRFB
SplÓ = ÒrfbsplÓ) signal that allows sampling of a new reconstructed target
signal; Target Change Detection also allows such sampling. The second output
from Saccade Enabling (third output from Defoveating Fast-Phase Generation) is
a fast-phase amplitude signal (ÒFPh SizeÓ = ÒfphszÓ) that is added to a
sampled, reconstructed retinal error signal to determine saccade size via the
sampled saccadic motor command (ÒSaccmcÓ = ÒsmcsprÓ) that is sent to the PG
(this is done in the ÒSaccade Motor CommandÓ block).
The
Enable Control circuitry uses sampled, reconstructed retinal error (after a 0.3
deg dead zone) (Òe'*DZÓ = ÒecdstrdzÓ), PG (ÒpgoutÓ), and Target Change
Detection (ÒTgt ch DetÓ = ÒtchngÓ) to determine if an ECPY (i.e.,
"corrective") saccade or a RetFB (i.e., "fixation") saccade
should be enabled. If "e'*DZ" is non-zero and it has been less than
150 ms since the last detected change in target position, PG is passed to the
output, "ECPY Tmng" (ÒecpyenabÓ). If, on the other hand, at least 150
ms has passed since the last detected target change, the "RetFB
En&Spl" = "rfbspl" output will go high (value = 1). The
"RetFB En&Spl" = "rfbenctl" passes to the Ret FB Enab
& Sample circuitry.
The
ECPY Timing circuitry acts on the "ECPY EnCntr" =
"ecenctrl" input signal. It outputs an "ECPY Enable" =
"ecpyenab" signal that is 10 ms long (set in "enable
duration") and starts 130 ms after PG concludes (set in "post-pulse
delay time"). The "ECPY Enable" signal passes to the RetFB
Enable & Sample circuitry.
The
RetFB Enable & Sample circuitry uses 5 inputs: retinal error position
(err50), "RetFB En&Spl" = "rfbenctrl" from the Enable
Control circuitry, "eedot" and "TIDZ" from the DFFPh Gen
circuitry, and "ecpyenab" from the ECPY Timing circuitry. Its outputs
are "RetFb Spl" = "rfbspl" and "RetFB Enable"
("sacenab"). The "rfbspl" signal allows sampling of
reconstructed target position (T') to produce (T'*). Each "ecpyenab"
input causes "rfbspl" to go to 0, where it remains until the RFB
latency of 200 ms expires (if no other "ecpyenab" signals occur) and
sets "rfbspl" to 1. "RetFb Spl" also goes to 1 with high
outputs (value = 1) of either of the two 4-input AND gates. Before a
"RetFB Enab" signal is created, 1 of 5 criteria must be satisfied,
several of which depend on specific combinations of the 5 inputs to the RetFB
Enable and Sample circuitry. Two criteria that directly trigger a "RetFB
Enable" output are a "RetFB En&Spl" signal ("rfbenctrl")
form the Enable Control circuitry and an "rfbspl" signal from within
this block. Each of 2 other criteria result from the outputs of the two 4-input
AND gates. The first AND gate requires that 4 conditions be met: 200 ms has
elapsed since the last "sacenab" signal, "TIDZ" must be 0,
"err50" must have a magnitude >0.5, and "eedot" must be
high (value = 1). The second AND gate also requires that 4 conditions be met:
200 ms has elapsed since the last "sacenab" signal, "err50"
has to be non-zero, "eedot" must be high (value = 1), and
"TIDZ" must be non-zero. The final criterion that triggers
"RetFB Enable" is the output of the 2-input AND gate when
"ecpyenab" and its second input are high (value = 1). That is, when
the magnitude of the retinal error is higher than the 0.5 threshold. This
triggers a "corrective" saccade.
The
Defoveating Fast Phase Generation circuitry uses 7 inputs: "tial",
"err50", "RetFB Enable" from the RetFB Enable & Sample circuitry
(ÒrfbenabÓ), "edot50", "ECPY Enable" from the ECPY Enable
circuitry (ÒecpyenabÓ), "sbsac", and TCD (ÒtchngÓ). Its outputs are:
"FPh size" = "fphsz", "eedot", and TIDZ. The
"FPh size" output sums with "e'*DZ" to determine the
magnitude of "saccmc" = "smcspr". The "eedot" and
"TIDZ" outputs are used by the RetFB Enable & Sample circuitry.
The "FPh size" output is "TIDZ" multiplied by -0.8 if
either "rfbenab" or ÒecpyenabÓ are high (value = 1) and at least 200
ms has elapsed since the last "tchng" signal; if both are low,
"FPh size" is 0. The "eedot" output is high if either
"err50" and "edot50" sampled are greater than 0 or both are
less than 0. As the name implies, the "TIDZ" output is created by
passing the "tial" input through a dead zone of 4 deg/sec.
Saccade
and Drift Blanking: The Saccade and Drift Blanking circuitry prevents other
logic from evaluating steady-state target, eye, or retinal variables during, or
immediately after saccades. It creates a blanking signal (ÒsbsacÓ) that lasts
for PW + 70 ms, using a delayed PG signal (ÒpgoutÓ delayed by 80 ms). The
"sbsac" signal is also used to prevent the effects of post-saccadic
drift from adversely affecting calculation of reconstructed target velocity.
The "sbsac" signal causes the "SP S/H" = "sbsp"
signal to be 0 (after "sbsac" goes to 1 after 80+PW+70 ms) until the
velocity error signal (ÒvelerrÓ) begins to increase, at which time
"sbsp" goes to 1.
Neural
Integrator Control: When TI is non-zero, the Neural Integrator Control
circuitry allows the NI to integrate the output of PG until its output (desired
eye position) is equal to the reconstructed target position. The output signal
(ÒHoldÓ = ÒniholdÓ) is used to remove the PG input from the NI via a control
switch. ÒHoldÓ is set to 0 when both PG and the reconstructed error signal
(ÒtpstarÓ – ÒeposprÓ) are non-zero. During PG, ÒHoldÓ remains low (value
= 0) until the reconstructed error signal crosses zero, whereupon ÒHoldÓ is set
high (value = 1). ÒHoldÓ is also set to 1 if PG terminates. In the absence of a
tonic imbalance (TI=0), the NI integrates all PG signals. Other conditions
(e.g., for GEN, SP, etc.) need to be added to activate this circuitry that
allows the NI to hold its value when it has arrived at the correct eye
position.
Alexander's
Law: The original circuitry (v1.0) used efference copy of eye position
(epospr) to modulate the TI input and produce "TIAL" =
"tial". In the present model (v1.4), a more stable version of the
efference copy of eye position (ÒtpstarÓ – ÒecdstrdzÓ, without nystagmus)
was used to simulate the gaze-angle effect of INS via ÒNullModÓ. The eye
position signal is multiplied by the Alexander's law slope and filtered before
summing with TI. Depending on the sign of TI, this sum is kept greater than or
less than 0 and passed onto the lower final switch that only passes an output
if TI is nonzero. In this version of the model (v1.4), when there is a TI input
(e.g., VN or FMNS), this gaze-angle modulation is turned off via the upper
penultimate switch.
Braking/Foveating
Saccade Logic: This circuitry (for INS) uses sampled, reconstructed retinal
position error (Òecd1strÓ), sampled, reconstructed retinal slip velocity
(ÒsvprÓ = Òslipvel2Ó), and desired eye velocity (ÒEvel'Ó) to determine if the
conditions for generating a BS are met. Braking saccades will always occur in
the direction opposite to eye motion if the eye is moving away from the target.
First, "ecd1str" is used by the BS or FS circuit to determine if
retinal error is increasing (calling for a BS) or decreasing (no BS). If this
criterion for a BS is met, its magnitude is determined within limits. Second,
"svpr" is compared to a threshold; if it exceeds it, the second
criterion for a BS is met. Third, the direction of "Evel'" is
determined and used to assign the direction of the BS. The product of these
logic signals is used with "Evel'" by the BS Enable circuit. If the
derivative of "Evel'" (desired eye acceleration) falls below
threshold, a BS is enabled for a period of time determined by the BS Timing circuit.
In this version (v1.4) we added reconstructed target velocity (ÒtvprÓ) as an
input to allow more accurate foveating saccades during smooth pursuit. This is
done by either adding or subtracting from the prior calculation depending on
the relative directions of the pursuit and the foveating saccade.
Saccade
Motor Command: This block generates the ultimate position motor command
(ÒposmcÓ) that PG sees and executes. ÒposmcÓ is generated by using a voluntary
saccade switch (Vol Sacc Sw) to sample the sum of ÒfphszÓ and ÒecdstrdzÓ. In
v1.4, ÒecdstrdzÓ is multiplied by the output of ÒCorrective Sacc InhibitionÓ,
which inhibits the unwanted voluntary saccades during foveation periods of
nystagmus while leaving the wanted voluntary saccades unchanged. The switch output
is an indication of the saccadic amplitude. It is held for 150 ms if target
position change is detected.
Model
Plant+: This block uses the slow and fast components of the efference copy
to reconstruct the plant output. In the previous release, the slow and fast
components were not distinguished. The structure of Model Plant+ is identical
to the modified double motor neuron plant. Òecpy80Ó is a delayed version
of the plant output (eye position
reconstructed); ÒimvelÓ is the differentiated reconstructed eye position, i.e.,
the reconstructed eye velocity. These two signals are used in multiple places
(mentioned above) for various logical operations in IM.
NYSTAGMUS
HYPOTHESES
INS
The
underlying hypothesis for INS is that it represents a failure to calibrate the
normal damping in the SP system, resulting in a continuous, sinusoidal velocity
oscillation (a high-gain instability). All subsequent known INS waveforms are
the result of the interactions as the normal saccadic subsystem attempting to
brake the oscillation, and the fixation system's attempts to foveate the
target.
FMNS
The
underlying hypothesis for FMNS is not restricted to any specific hypothesis for
its genesis. The driving force for FMNS is considered to be a tonic imbalance
that results from either of the three current hypotheses for FMNS. It emanates
from the visuovestibular system (not included in this version) and is a tonic
imbalance (TI) that drives the eyes off target with constant velocity slow
phases. The decelerating slow phases also observed in FMNS are produced by a
switch in saccadic function from foveation of the target to defoveation when
the TI becomes too large (more than 4 deg/sec) for good acuity. That allows the
fixation system to slow the drift for better acuity.
FUTURE
DEVELOPMENT
We
continue to expand the current OMS model (v1.4) and will release future
versions after they are tested. Currently the pursuit-response simulations are
for ramp or step-ramp target inputs. The pursuit system will be updated for the
post-tenotomy responses for greater accuracy. This update requires further
study of post-tenotomy pursuit data and the plasticity of the OMS after
extraocular muscle surgeries. Also, pursuit and gaze-angle variation will be
coordinated to work synergistically. The characteristic of a moving Ònull,Ó
when the eyes are moving in response to a moving target or head movement, makes
this revision of the model more challenging. Additionally, more human data
should be observed to determine if the model should also be tweaked so that
responses to high-velocity targets are more accurate.
If
users find any bugs, they are encouraged to bring them to our attention so
that, after testing them, we may incorporate them into future versions (with
citation of the source). In this way we hope to expand the behaviors of the
model as well as the ocular motor deficits it can simulate. Changes made by
users without consultation and testing by the Daroff-DellÕOsso Ocular Motility
Lab should be identified as such in any publication.
We
will also incorporate different versions of key functional blocks as
neurophysiological evidence is provided in the literature. We hope this model
will become a platform for testing putative models of subfunctions that come
out of research into brain-site specific, single-cell recordings. The model has
already demonstrated its predictive value for static and dynamic behavioral
effects of tenotomy surgery in INS (13,14). It had also proven useful in teaching
normal and abnormal ocular motility as its outcomes can be made into QuickTime
movies and incorporated into programs designed for computer projection of
figures and movies.
REFERENCES
1. Dell'Osso
LF. Nystagmus basics. Normal models that simulate dysfunction. In: Hung GK,
Ciuffreda KJ, eds. Models of the Visual System. New York: Kluwer Academic / Plenum Publishers, 2002; 711-39.
2. Jacobs
JB, Dell'Osso LF. Congenital nystagmus: hypothesis for its genesis and complex
waveforms within a behavioral ocular motor system model. JOV
2004; 4(7):604-25.
3. Rucker
JC, Dell'Osso LF, Jacobs JB, Serra A. "Staircase" saccadic intrusions
plus transient yoking and neural integrator failure associated with cerebellar
hypoplasia: a model simulation. Sem
Ophthalmol 2006; 21:229-43.
4. Eberhorn
AC, Horn AKE, Fischer P, BŸttner-Ennever JA. Proprioception and pallisade
endinge in extraocular eye muscles. In: Ramat S, Straumann D, eds. Clinical
and Basic Oculomotor Research. In Honor of David S. Zee—Ann NY Acad Sci
1039. New York: NYAS, 2005; 1-8.
5. Robinson
DA. A note on the oculomotor pathway. Exp
Neurol 1968; 22:130-2.
6. Dell'Osso
LF, Jacobs JB. A normal ocular motor system model that simulates the dual-mode
fast phases of latent/manifest latent nystagmus. Biological Cybernetics
2001; 85:459-71.
7. Wang
ZI, Dell'Osso LF, Jacobs JB. Expanding the original behavioral infantile
nystagmus syndrome model to jerk waveforms and gaze-angle variations. In: Leigh
RJ, Devereaux MW, eds. Advances in Understanding Mechanisms and Treatment of
Congenital Forms of Nystagmus. New York:
Oxford University Press, 2008; 139-42.
8. Wang
Z, Dell'Osso LF, Zhang Z, Leigh RJ, Jacobs JB. Tenotomy does not affect
saccadic velocities: Support for the "small-signal" gain hypothesis. Vision Res 2006; 46:2259-67.
9. Wang
Z, Dell'Osso LF, Jacobs JB, Burnstine RA, Tomsak RL. Effects of tenotomy on
patients with infantile nystagmus syndrome: foveation improvement over a
broadened visual field. JAAPOS 2006; 10:552-60.
10. Wang
ZI, Dell'Osso LF, Tomsak RL, Jacobs JB. Combining recessions (nystagmus and
strabismus) with tenotomy improved visual function and decreased oscillopsia
and diplopia in acquired downbeat nystagmus and in horizontal infantile
nystagmus syndrome. JAAPOS 2007; 11:135-41.
11. Robinson
DA, Gordon JL, Gordon SE. A model of smooth pursuit eye movements. Biol Cyber 1986; 55:43-57.
12. Abel
LA, Dell'Osso LF, Daroff RB. Analog model for gaze-evoked nystagmus. IEEE Trans Biomed Engng 1978; BME(25):71-5.
13. Wang
ZI, Dell'Osso LF. Being "slow to see" is a dynamic visual function
consequence of infantile nystagmus syndrome: Model predictions and patient data
identify stimulus timing as its cause. Vision
Res 2007; 47(11):1550-60.
14. Wang
ZI, Dell'Osso LF. Tenotomy procedure alleviates the "slow to see"
phenomenon in infantile nystagmus syndrome: model prediction and patient data. Vision Res 2008; 48:1409-19.
Although the
information contained in this paper and its downloading are free, please
acknowledge its source by citing the paper as follows:
Wang, Z.I. and DellÕOsso, L.F.: Behavioral OMS Model v1.4. OMLAB Report #070108, 1-16,
2008. http://www.omlab.org/Teaching/teaching.html
APPENDIX
OMS Model Versions
OMSv1.mdl: Original model as presented in JOV paper
[Released on web site 7/30/04]
OMSv1_1.mdl: OMSv1.0 model with combined IM components
[Changes: IM]
OMSv1_1_SSI.mdl: OMSv1.1 model with staircase saccadic intrusions
[Changes: Test bed, Sampled Target & Error Reconstruction]
OMSv1_2.mdl: OMSv1.1 model with OMS Model Lib available
[Changes: Model Lib]
OMSv1_2_1.mdl: OMSv1.1 model with OMS Model Lib available
[Changes: PG (New rNI), Model Lib]
OMSv1_3.mdl:
OMSv1.2 model with:
1) Improved SP
accuracy of INS foveating saccades
[Changes: IM,
BS/FS Logic]
2)
AL-controlled INS nulls
[Changes:
IM, AL, PMC+]
OMSv1_3_1.mdl: OMSv1.2.1 model with:
1) Improved SP
accuracy of INS foveating saccades
[Changes: IM,
BS/FS Logic]
2)
AL-controlled INS nulls
[Changes:
IM, AL, PMC+]
OMSv1_4.mdl: OMSv1.3
model with dual-signal OMNs and Plant
1) OMNs for SEM +
saccades and SEM only
[Changes: OMN,
Model, Plant, Model Plant]
2)
Plant pathways for SEM + saccades and SEM only
[Changes:
Plant, Model Plant]
3)
Distributed final 30 ms delay
[Changes:
pre-Plant, Model+ pre-Plant, Model, Sacc Enab,
Trgt Chng Det,
Trgt Recon, BS/FS Logic, Slip Vel Recon]
4)
New PG with new rNI to avoid error messages of original rNI
[Changes: PG (New
rNI, altered gains and non-linearity in
PW, altered PH
table), Model Lib (New PG lib)]
5)
Plant with SEM gain adjustment (EOM surgery)
[Changes:
Plant, Model Plant]
6)
Inhibit corrective saccades if fs
[Changes:
Sacc Mot Cmd, Corr Sacc Inhib]
7)
Modified Fixation system to prevent accumulated error
[Changes:
Fixation, Fix Lib]
8)
Provided a cleaner signal of eye position for AL modulation
[Changes:
AlexanderÕs Law]
9)
Ensured no fsmc30 for 250 ms after Target change
[Changes:
BSFS Logic]
[Released on web site 7/1/08]